List
Facebook Twitter Reddit Tumblr Email

Some police departments are turning to wearable cameras, allowing their officers to record interactions with citizens. At the Taser International headquarters in Scottsdale, Ariz., Joseph LeDuc, a police officer, checked a video made with such a camera. (Photo: Joshua Lott, The New York Times)

Those of us who don’t confront the potential wide diffusion of on-officer body-worn cameras with excitement and hopefulness have already grown accustomed to some pat responses from advocates. Certain to be among them is the citation of a study from Rialto, California, that has made national news.

As the New York Times reported:

The Rialto study began in February 2012 and will run until this July [2013]. The results from the first 12 months are striking. Even with only half of the 54 uniformed patrol officers wearing cameras at any given time, the department over all had an 88 percent decline in the number of complaints filed against officers, compared with the 12 months before the study, to 3 from 24.

Rialto’s police officers also used force nearly 60 percent less often — in 25 instances, compared with 61. When force was used, it was twice as likely to have been applied by the officers who weren’t wearing cameras during that shift, the study found.

After completion of the study, Britain’s The Guardian relayed the same statistics and reported “Rialto’s randomised controlled study has seized attention because it offers scientific – and encouraging – findings.” Civilian police monitoring groups, like Police The Police, touted the new technology with an internet meme that circulated widely on social media.

Image: Police The Police

New York district court judge cited the Rialto study when she found unconstitutional the New York Police Department policy of “stop-and-frisk.” It seemed anyone concerned with policing and the use of force by officers was paying attention to Rialto. “I think we’ve opened some eyes in the law enforcement world. We’ve shown the potential,” study author, Tony Farrar, told The Guardian. “It’s catching on.”

Farrar says the study was conducted to measure the deterrence effect on use-of-force incidence. He explains,

The cameras were hypothesized to increase police officers’ self-consciousness and, therefore, increase their compliance to rules of conduct, especially concerning use of force. The findings suggest a more than 50 percent reduction in the total number of incidents of use of force compared to control-conditions, and nearly 10 times less citizens’ complaints than in the 12 months prior to the experiment.

The study claims officers randomly selected by shift to wear cameras ended up using force half as frequently as those without the cameras.

The report concludes with an important limitation most news reports and advocates for on-officer cameras ignore. Farrar encouraged caution for those basing policy recommendations on the study, explaining, “leaders cannot rule out the possibility that the cameras have (also) modified the behavior of those who interacted with the police. Members of the public with whom the officers communicated were also aware of being videotaped and, therefore, were likely to be cognizant that they ought to act cooperatively.” Because Farrar’s main research claim was that knowledge of being monitored changes behavior, he presumed this would apply symmetrically to officers and the civilians they stopped and subsequently notified they were being recorded. As such, this complicates simple, causative claims circulated in media reports.

Most news reports are clear in noting the Rialto study author, Tony Farrar, is also the Chief of Police for the Rialto police departent. However, few explain this as a potential conflict of interest or a methodological problem. Farrar’s study was conducted as part of his criminology Masters research at Cambridge University. He was, importantly, simultaneously was the head of the agency and managed both complaint review processes and those officers involved in the study.

Since Farrar’s hypothesis was that behaviors change when people know they are being carefully monitored, not only the cameras, but also the organizational authority of the individual designing and implementing the experimental study were variables.

Farrar’s research report and media articles relaying findings to public audiences fail to acknowledge Farrar’s role beyond that of an observer. He presumed a “view from nowhere” that necessarily troubles any research project. This is especially troubling, however, where the researcher is also a superior in a command-style bureaucracy. His position was hardly that of a neutral observer. Why, out of dozens of media reports, this is not raised as a problem is worth questioning. (I asked the NYT reporter if he holds investments in Taser, manufacturer of the AXON Flex wearable camera system. Obviously offended by the question, he nonetheless replied he does not.)

Farrar was doubly committed to his hypothesis proving true, both as a researcher and as a publicly accountable police administrator. But in the Rialto case, this is especially punctuated. The city of Rialto was ready to disband the police department and turn law enforcement functions over to the county sheriff. Farrar was hired as a reformer in a last ditch effort to save the department and the jobs of all the officers and staff who worked there. A department troubled by a history of questionable use of force—so questionable that it posed an existential threat to it—had to demonstrate reduced use of force and other complaints or face elimination.

Farrar had a chance to demonstrate his capability as a police administrator—not only to local policy-makers, but to his criminology research committee examining him for a graduate degree.

Research on the efficacy of policy aimed at reducing use of force is often equivocal or tentative. Researchers do mostly agree that if policy is effective, it is because of the buy-in of managers, administrators, and officers. So we should expect that something would change when the head of a department is deeply committed to a policy designed to reforming use of force among its officers. However, the cameras are getting all the credit in this study.

6 Responses to “Cameras on Cops and Junk Science in Rialto”

  1. William B. Scott

    Great additional insight to this study. Cameras MAY reduce the number of officer-involved shootings, but this study is hardly “proof.” My son’s murder-by-cop was captured on a Costco security video, but cops and Costco personnel intentionally destroyed data that would have proven THEY screwed up and literally executed Erik Scott for having a BlackBerry phone in his hand. Then they covered up critical evidence to exonerate dirty, corrupt cops and a complicit Costco employee, who caused my son’s death. Bizarre? Yes, but true. Read all about it a novel of “faction,” entitled “The Permit.”

  2. William B. Scott

    Great additional insight to this study. Cameras MAY reduce the number of officer-involved shootings, but this study is hardly “proof.” My son’s murder-by-cop was captured on a Costco security video, but cops and Costco personnel intentionally destroyed data that would have proven THEY screwed up and literally executed Erik Scott for having a BlackBerry phone in his hand. Read about it in “The Permit,” a work of “faction.”

  3. When's It Gonna Stop? - If The River Swells

    […] likely continue to see these cameras “malfunction” during exceptionally brutal acts. There is an oft-cited study claiming that body cameras cut down on use of force. This actually was conducted by the chief of a […]

  4. Cameras on Cops and Junk Science in Rialto | Surveillance State

    […] From Ben Brucato […]

  5. We must disband the police: Body cameras aren’t enough — only radical change will stop cops who kill | Prison Books Collective

    […] insists that there’s no firm basis for widespread claims about bodycam effectiveness. Others have debunked the weak science of the study, showing moreover that the increased visibility provided nationally by bodycams and […]

  6. javier de rivera

    Cool, that is an amaizing post (BTW, I’m not a bot!)

    It shows how the falacy of attributing agency to technological devices is used to mask the real relevance of the human factor behind those devices. That way people get confused with details, while the real issue is the personal determination of the people involved (and in charge).

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

  Posts

1 4 5 6 7
September 28th, 2011

Phenomenal experiences of technology

In “Feelings and phenomenal experiences” by Schwarz and Clore, in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, they discuss the role […]

September 28th, 2011

The material and political confinement of social constructions

Between the politics of technology and the social construction of technological systems (SCOTS)[1], exists considerable tension over three distinct problems […]

August 2nd, 2011

Dispatches from the Decade of the Leak: The Antisec retaliation for Anonymous arrests

“I do not believe in leaks. I would execute leakers. They’re betraying our country.” –Ralph Peters, U.S. Army Lieutenant Colonel […]

April 8th, 2011

The Decade of the Leak

After the first major Wikileaks release and the subsequent “manhunt” for Julian Assange, I dubbed this the Decade of the […]

January 23rd, 2011

Review of “Grassroots Postmodernism” by Esteva and Prakash

Esteva and Prakash’s Grassroots Postmodernism presents a powerful theoretical model for alternatives to development.  In reading this accessible, yet deep […]

December 19th, 2010

Jeph Jerman: calling out the voice of animated nature

Jeph Jerman could be described as an electro-acoustic artist, an experimental musician, or an avant-garde performer.  But what Jeph is, […]

May 12th, 2010

Personality Online: Anonymous, Toxic and Otherwise Destructive

I’ve been intensively studying the literature on how technology changes society. My focus has been on technologies much more simple […]

February 22nd, 2010

Post-development theory, alternatives to development and activist anthropology

In “Anthropology and the Development Encounter,” Arturo Escobar discusses the past approaches of development anthropology as problematic.  He focuses on […]

December 3rd, 2009

Characterizing a paradigm shift: The UN discourse on sustainable development as the greening of globalism

Below is the introduction to a 15,000 essay I just completed, summing up the theoretical and historical basis for my […]

November 14th, 2009

Quote from “Sustainable Development and Agenda 21” by Timothy Doyle

When I was researching for my work on Agenda 21 and the UNCED, I found very little wholesale criticism in […]